Showing posts with label English Learning with N2s. Show all posts
Showing posts with label English Learning with N2s. Show all posts

Saturday, August 21, 2010

Linguistic Problems and Complexities

Linguistic Problems and Complexities
Linguistic problems and complexities can be classed as lexical, syntactic or semantic depending on their context. Lexical problems involve the interpretation of particular words or phrases rather than entire classes. These problems exist independent of context although they are only evident in it. Syntactic problems involve structural relations between words or phrases; they are often expressed semantically (ie in ambiguity) but this is a symptom rather than a cause.
Semantic problems are subdivided into lexical, syntactic and discourse types. Although both lexical and semantic lexical problems involve single words or phrases, semantic problems are syncategoric rather than specific. Semantic problems in syntax occur when a construction's syntax is correct but its sense is ill-formed or ambiguous, or vice versa. Discourse semantic cases involve an utterance's discourse context and, because utterances of this type are both syntactically and semantically well-formed, describe language complexities rather than problems.
Some of these cases can be handled by existing processing methods while others remain intractable. A few examples are not problems at all but are included simply because they give insight into the nature of the English language. Most are borrowed or adapted from other sources (Hirst, 1987; Leech, 1981; Marcus, 1978; Milne, 1986; Pereira & Warren, 1980; Perrault, 1985; Rettig, 1988; Ross, 1967; Winograd, 1983). Discussion are preceded by examples to which reference may be made by a parenthesized index.
Lexical Cases
Lexical Ambiguity
in syntactic category (part of speech)
`sink', `saw', `club', `ring'
A sink is a -plumbing-fixture- noun as well as a verb that means to -disappear-underwater-. Syntactically ambiguous words can in addition be semantically ambiguous within a given category. As a noun, `club' is a homonym for both -bludgeoning-weapon- and -recreational-association-; within the latter sense, it is polysemous because it can mean both recreational-social-group and -recreational-building-. Taken as a set the examples suggest that syntactically or semantically ambiguous words are often common and short.
in word denotation
`Baker',`Quebec'
These words denote a man or a role (1), or a city or province (2). Denotational ambiguity can affect quantification; a baker is a general role but Baker is a specific person.
in abbreviation denotation
`Jan', `MA', `BC'
Respectively, these particles denote a name or a month, a degree or a state, and a province or a period of historical time. Abbreviations differ from words in being able to incorporate punctuation and capitalization which can help distinguish between their different meanings. These aids however may be missing, partial or nonstandard.
2. Lexical Gaps
`oppression', `aggression'; `oppress', *`aggress'
The first two words both appear to be nominalizations but no form of `aggression' exists to match the verb `oppress'. This phenomenon of a lack of a word to express a concept is called a lexical gap. Lexical gaps are generally understood to apply to easily understood concepts having collateral concepts which are realized in words.
3. Unrecognized Words
`blivit', `snood', `chela'.
People routinely encounter words which they don't know or which are meaningless.
4. Idioms
`redeye', `tie one on', `carry coals to Newcastle', `yuppie', `ayatollah of popular culture', `xerox'
Idioms come in all sizes--words, phrases and clauses. They can include coinages (1, 4) and proper names (3) and may have relatively short lifespans (4, 5). Idioms that survive long enough pass into the language permanently (6).
5. Subidiomatic Expressions
`just about', "We were just about to leave"; `enough' +inf, "old enough to know better but young enough to do it".
In addition to idioms based on nouns and verbs or those with substantial concrete sense, there exists a class of subidiomatic expressions which act as facilitators of expression or linguistic shortcuts. These expressions are usually based on modifiers (3) and may have co-occurring parts of speech (4). A subidiom's sense limited and is homologous to the sense of the modifier or the part with which it occurs.
Syntactic Cases
6. Center Embedding
"The wallpaper the man your son suggested put up is crooked", "The rat the cat the dog bit chased ate the cheese".
This well-formed sentence is difficult to understand because encountering a subordinate clause forces a reader to suspend his partial understanding of the entire sentence. The possibility of confusing contexts arises as soon as two are stacked.
7. Garden Path Sentences
"The horse raced past the barn door fell", "The man who whistles tunes pianos", "The old man the boat", "The cotton clothing is made of grows in Mississippi", "Have the students who failed the exam take the supplementary".
Garden path sentences employ words which can be understood as more than one part of speech. These categorically-ambiguous words appear both within clauses (3 & 5) and between clauses (1, 2 & 4). The punctuation and relative pronoun introducers which would unambiguously identify a clausal reading are elided. Taking the examples in order, `raced' can act as the head of an adjective phrase and as a past participle, `tunes' and `man' can be noun or verb, `cotton' noun or adjective, and `have' main or auxiliary verb in imperative or interrogative constructions.
Garden paths often contain intervening phrases or clauses which postpone a reader's revision of his misinterpretation and therefore encourage misunderstanding. Removing an intervening construction can sometimes eliminate an alternative reading and always makes the main clause easier to comprehend. When this is performed on the four examples of this sort, "Have the students take the supplementary", "The man tunes pianos" and "The cotton grows in Mississippi" immediately becomes unambiguous. "The horse raced fell" becomes ungrammatic; only the presence of the interjected prepositional phrase `past the barn door' allows the adjective to be postposed legitimately. After paraphrasing it becomes "The raced horse fell", which is unambiguous.
8. Extensive Clause Ordination (run-on sentences)
"This is the cat that bit the rat that ate the cheese that Jack bought", "They came for dinner and they stayed all night and they left right after breakfast but they were still late and were punished when they got to school".
There is no formal difficulty in processing these sentences but in practice they become increasingly onerous to handle as they go on. It appears that we use sentence terminal punctuation as a signal to chunk our understanding. When a sentence ends, we know that we can stop watching out for an entire class of possible syntactic relationships redeploy our attention to the next series of words.
9. Syntactic Ambiguity
in modifier scope
`the old men and women'
Only context can tell whether this phrase means `old men and old women', or `old men and women of unspecified age'.
in phrase reference
"I saw him on the hill with a telescope"; "He worked on the table on the paper", "He worked on the paper on the table".
The first sentence has three readings which depend on how the last prepositional phrase is interpreted. If `with a telescope' is read as accompaniment, it can modify `him' or `hill' and produce "he and a telescope were seen on the hill" or "he was seen on the hill which has a telescope on it". If the phrase is taken as identifying the instrument used for seeing, it produces "he was seen on the hill by use of a telescope".
The second and third sentences show how ambiguity arises when a sentence contains more than one phrase which can play more than one available role. Selecting one role for the first phrase automatically assigns the second to the other phrase. Thus either sentence can be interpreted as saying that a paper is being written by someone sitting at a table, or that a table is being painted or sanded on top of spread paper. `At' can be substituted for `on' anywhere.
in coordinated conjunction
`the men and women or boys'
Depending upon how the scope of the conjunctions is interpreted, this phrase can mean `either men and women, or men and boys', or `either men and women, or boys'.
in indirect object sense
"John wrote Mary a letter", "We envy you your wealth".
The indirect object is inherently semantically ambiguous. It almost always expresses senses of -directed-towards- and on-behalf-of and is paraphrased as `to' +indobj or `for' +indobj. Although context within a sentence usually establishes what sense is intended, this example proves that it is possible to make ambiguous statements; it is not clear whether the letter was written to Mary or on her behalf.
The second sentence is an example of a sentence which cannot be paraphrased in either of the two standard ways. The `to' reading makes no sense. "We envy you for your wealth" makes sense but is not quite correct. Envying someone for wealth is not the same as wishing to possess the wealth itself, which is what the second example really means.
in contracted possessives
"I have John's picture"
This suggests either that I have a picture belonging to John, or that I have a picture of him. Ambiguity arises if a form expresses two different senses by virtue of contraction, and disappears when the contraction is expanded. Thus "I have a picture of John's" and "I have a picture of John" each unambiguously express one of the example's readings.
in part of speech
"John performed the woman moves in the folk dance after he taught the woman moves in the opening number. See how quickly the woman moves now"; "They're cooking apples"; "Time flies like an arrow"; "I know that boy is bad", "I know that boys are bad"; "What boys did it?", "What boys do is not my business"; "The trash can be smelly", "The trash can was smelly"; "I told the girl that I liked the story", "I told the girl who I liked the story", "I told the girl the story that I liked".
This is essentially the same problem as lexical ambiguity in syntactic case. However these examples go beyond identifying the problem to demonstrate it. Each `girl moves' collocation in the first sentence is composed of different parts of speech; ?adj+noun, noun+noun, noun+verb. `Cooking' acts as a present participle and an adjective (2). The third example is a classic with five readings, only four of which I can identify: flies should be timed a certain way, a type of fly likes arrows, time passes quickly, and ?it's time that flies liked arrows.
10. Ellipsis
"She dances better than he does", "I did it; he didn't".
A literal reading of either sentence asks the question: than he does what? Ellipsis within a sentence can frequently be identified by the presence of coordinating conjunctions or punctuation between an elliptical clause and its referent. Elliptical reference between sentences frequently relies on structural symmetry between the two related expressions.
11. Cross-serial Agreement
"This land and these woods can be expected to rent itself and sell themselves respectively", "The last two people in this picture live in Columbus and Chicago"
Sentences using `respectively' explicitly or implicitly coordinate on a cross-serial basis (rather than on a center-embedding one). In cross-serial coordination, each conjunct in a series is linked with its counterpart in the same position in another series. The construction is confusing when the number of conjuncts does not agree. More than two series can be coordinated; (1) is still easily understood if `in 1989 and 1990' is appended.
12. Anaphora
`do this', `such things', `doing so';
"Jane gave Joan candy because she was hungry", "Jane gave Joan candy because she wasn't hungry";
"A woman and a girl each had a hat and the woman gave hers to the girl".
Although anaphora usually involve references to nouns (`this'), they can also apply to adjectives (`such') or adverbs (`so'). Reference in the next example can only be determined by pragmatic knowledge of who was hungry. In terms of the last example, which hat was transferred in each of the following sentences: 1) "The girl gave hers to the woman"; 2) "The girl gave hers back to the woman"; 3) "The girl gave it back to the woman"?
13. Multiple Word Nouns and Merged Noun Compounds
`pipe wrench', `putty knife', `shoe horn', `property tax office furniture', `horse collar'.
Phrases which contain more than one noun in their head can be divided into two types. In multiple word noun phrases, the last noun supplies the dominant elements of the phrase's sense and each preceding noun specifies and extends it (1, 2, 4). Merged noun compounds are concatenations of nouns which give rise to a new sense largely unrelated to those of its components (3, 5).
14. Preposed Phrases
"For each person there, I had a gift".
This slightly unusual construction emphasizes the opening phrase. There are no problems in processing, particularly since the presence of structural punctuation signals the likelihood of distance dependency (between `had' and `for').
15. Syntactically Ill-formed Input
"The boys is here". "Hey. You. Yes, you. Do it. That's right. Right. No. Okay"
Ungrammatical utterances will always be with us. They are found particularly in speech, where the parallel visual and aural modes act to integrate the entire communication and to substitute for broken phraseology and incomplete expression. Ill-formed statements can be divided into ones which are completely but incorrectly formed and those which are much less complete than the usual elliptical statement.
16. Nominals
"I regretted his going", "His going surprised me", ?"After his going we hired an athlete.", ??"His going's main effect was to end the feud"
Nominals become more awkward as the construction in which they appear becomes more complex. Ross attributes this awkwardness to the `nouniness' which a nominalized form acquires as a result of its transformation (Ross, 1967 in Winograd, 1983).
17. Invalid Transformations
interrogative
"Merry wanted to see Barbara", "Who did Merry want to see?"; "Merry has a book that pleased Barbara", *"Who does Merry have a book that pleased?"
Transforming the first sentence into the second poses no problem, however executing a parallel transformation on the third statement generates an ill-formed sentence. The third sentence must be paraphrased to merge the subordinate clause into the main clause, generating "Barbara was pleased by Merry's book", before it can be transformed into an equivalent interrogative construction "Who was pleased by Merry's book?".
reflexive
"I screamed for Harry to open the door", *"I tried for me to open the door", ??"I tried for myself to open the door", "I tried to open the door for myself".
The first sentence is legitimate but a similar construction incorporating a reflexive relationship is ill-formed. The remaining sentences are paraphrases of the second statement that move toward correct syntax while retaining the reflexive element and the same sense. These examples seems to highlight an unmotivated complexity of English syntax.
18. Ergative Verbs
"The man is opening the door", "The man is laughing at the joke"; "The man is cooking dinner", "The man is cooking", "The meat is cooking"; "The water tastes bad", "I taste the water"
To establish a context for evaluating these unusual verbs, consider the first two non-ergative sentences which are respectively transitive and intransitive. Truncation produces one satisfactory outcome, "The man is laughing" and a dubious one, ?"The man is opening" (unless a context has previously been established).
Now consider the next three sentences. Although the surface syntax of these examples suggests that `cook' is bitransitive, taking or leaving an object equally well, in reality the verb seems to have an inherent transitivity. Performing the same truncation on (3) to produce (4) changes its sense; (4) is ambiguous, its different senses being -cooking-something- and -being-cooked-. This change in meaning did not occur with the first two truncations.
Verbs of this sort are called ergative. They seem to have such a great need for an object that if none is supplied syntactically, the action can be interpreted as rebounding on the agent. A ergative verb can meaningfully fill the two frames "Something is " and "Something is being "-it is unusual for the active and passive voices of a statement to express the same sense. Ergative sentences like (4) are therefore often ambiguous. Some instances require discourse context, while others can be disambiguated using only the features of the constituents. There is no ambiguity about (5) because -cooking-something- requires a human agent and `meat' is not a human being.
Taste', `smell' and other verbs describing perceptual processes are ergative. The mental sphere in particular the emotional is rife with instances of the sort of situation that ergative verbs express. Consider `fear' and `frighten'. Although both statements in the pair "He frightened me", "I feared him" are active, they refer to one act, not two . The factors operating here seem in some way to be akin to the two of the types of speech act, locutionary ("I feared him") and perlocutionary ("he frightened me"), generalized from cases of deliberate action to all acts.
19. Homomorphy (similar syntax, different semantics)
"Falling planes can be dangerous", "Flying planes can be dangerous", "Buying planes can be dangerous"; "Warren is eager to please", "Warren is easy to please".
The first set of examples use ergative verbs to illustrate homomorphy and the second set show that it is something other than ergation. (1) and (3) can be paraphrased without ambiguity as "Planes which are falling can be dangerous" and " buying planes can be dangerous". The second sentence can be read both ways: "Planes which are flying can be dangerous" and " flying planes can be dangerous". It seems to lack a constituent which would distinguish between the two senses. The paraphrases suggest that the confusion arises from the different senses of ergative verbs; "Planes which are being flown can be dangerous" is a near paraphrase of " flying planes can be dangerous".
The same distinction appears in the second example without the presence of ambiguity. A paraphrase of (4) makes `Warren' the direct object in " is easy to please Warren." while in (5) `Warren' is a complemented subject because `easy to please' is an idiom meaning `easily pleased'. Are there any other constructions which employ complete transformations like from adj+inf to adv+part?
20. Long Range Reference
"The man left the house. The sun was shining. It was warm and windless. A bird sang. He felt good", "He is the man who in the fullness of time and after talking to all of us who take an interest in these matters when the issue becomes important did it".
In the first example the correct reference for `he' must be found across three intervening sentences. Sentences frequently make references to neighboring or more remote statements and these references can easily be misunderstood. In the second example long range reference occurs within a sentence across a 24-word interjection between `who' and `did it".
References can also occur between noun and noun; we are expected to recognize entities which have been previously introduced into a narrative. Common noun references are harder to handle than a pronoun one because the latter obviously requires a referent whereas the former may be the introduction of a new actor. The problem is compounded when the reference is not direct but through a synonym.
21. Comparison
"Tide is better", "Tide is better than ABC", "Tide is better than any soap on sale in this country"; "Tide is the best soap on sale in this country".
All comparisons, whether comparative (1) or superlative (4), need to have their context specified. In the comparative case, this means identifying the particular individual (2) or set of individuals to which the subject is being compared (3). When the comparative evaluation is made against a set, the resulting statement is easily paraphrased into a superlative comparison. Thus (3) is a paraphrase of (4).
Lexical Semantic Cases
22. Literal vs Idiomatic Use
"I turn on the dias", "I turn on the tap";
"The men agreed to play ball",
"The men agreed to play ball Tuesday nights",
"Acknowledging their other differences, the men agreed to play ball on this issue".
Each word in the first sentence executes a traditional syntactic function; `on' heads a prepositional phrase that identifies where the rotation takes place. In the second sentence, `turn on' is a idiom in which the particle `on' indicates the direction of rotation. (4) and (5) show how the same sentence can be modified to select between its literal and idiomatic senses.
There is a dilemma here. A parser which treats (2) as a verb+prep will misunderstand it, while always taking the longest possible string from input (ie idiomatic use) means that (1) will never be correctly recognized.
23. Intensive vs Extensive Usage
"The president is at Camp David",
"The president is elected every four years".
Sometimes the name of a role is intended to identify the individual who currently fill it, and sometimes the role itself. This parallels the distinction between a term's intension and its extension.
24. Universal vs Specific Quantification
"I am looking for a man",
"The golden tamarind is smaller than a cat but twice as cute",
"I am holding a man",
"The man who sees me left the room",
"The man who saw me left the room".
The first example illustrates the problem; it is uncertain whether the sought man is a particular individual or simply any man at all. The second and third examples demonstrate that both definite and indefinite quantifiers can experience this kind of ambiguity. `A cat' is .universal and `the tamarind' may be; `a man' however must be interpreted specifically because no one cannot hold all men. The fourth and fifth sentences show that quantificational ambiguity can arise in tensed verbs. Given that I am within a room, a man's seeing me must precede his leaving the room. Therefore `sees' can only be understood as the generic sees-on-a-regular-basis rather than the specific single-act-of-sight. Because both verbs in the fifth sentence are past tense, no temporal order is established and `saw' can refer to either quantification.
No general method of resolving quantificational ambiguity has been discovered. However if a quantified phrase is highly specified by definition or through context, it is reasonable to assume that reference is to a particular entity. Thus in the absence of contrary context (such as reading the classified ads) "I am looking for a red 1977 Mustang" suggests that the speaker seeks a particular car known to himself. Similarly "The man who saw me for three weeks at 9 o'clock left the room" suggests that the seeing is an act unrelated to the leaving.
25. Ambiguity
inherent (polysemy and homonymy)
`right'
Words can be both polysemous and homonymous. `Right' has homonymous senses of -righteous- and -right-handed-. Each of these senses is itself polysemous. When employed in "the right thing to do" and "do right", -righteous- has homologs of correct-under-the-circumstances and -absolutely-correct-. The homologs of -right-handed- are demonstated in `on the right' and `my right hand'. They are somewhere-to-the-right and on-the-right-side-of-my-body.
`Right' can also occupies different syntactic categories: "Might makes right" (noun), "I right the bottle" (verb), `the right stuff' (adjective) and "Do right" (adverb)
auto-antonymy
"example"
A subset of ambiguous words carry pairs of senses which are anotnyms. This phenomenon is known by a variety of names including autoantonymy. See discussion on the LINGUIST list, early 1995 onwards.
in quantifier sense
"List the trains that service every city", "Not everyone came", "Everyone doesn't do that".
These sentences introduce ambiguity because `every' means both the universal quantifier `all' and the existential quantifier `some'. As a result the first example leaves it unclear whether the trains have to visit all cities or just one of them to be listed. The next examples show that negation can interact with quantifier ambiguity. `Not everyone' is existentially `not some one' and therefore someone, and universally `not all ones' and therefore no one.
Syntactic Semantic Cases
26. Ungrammaticality (to aid communication)
"Everyone loves his wife",
"Although it was predicted Goldwater didn't win",
"The man who shows he wants it will get the prize he deserves".
In each of these examples structure has been subordinated to communication; each is readily understood although it infringes on the rules of grammar. The first example would be more correctly stated as "Everyone loves their wives", which would introduce a syntactic ambiguity about the number of wives each person has.
The reference for `it' in (2) is an elided `his win' phrase or `that he would win' clause. The presence of either makes an awkward construction: "Although it was predicted that he would win, Goldwater didn't (win)"; "Although his win was predicted, Goldwater didn't (win)". The third case is similar to the second. It can be paraphrased as "If a man deserves a prize and shows that he wants it, he will receive the prize" but this fails to express the need to show desire emphasized in (3).
27. Passivization Effect
"Few people read many books", "Many books are read by few people";
"Some books are read by few people", "Few people read some books";
"No books are read by all people", "All people read no books".
In the first example (1) emphasizes the scarcity of voracious readers while its passive transformation (2) emphasizes the plenitude of poorly-read books. A sentence's first phrase seems to enjoy a position of importance. (1) and (2) make different statements because the focus of emphasis shifts from `books' to `readers' when voice is transformed from passive to active. Although the problem disappears when quantification is shifted to the neutral `some' in the second example (3, 4), `some' in this case appears to be a qualifier of identity rather than a quantifier of measure because the problem persists when the quantifiers are universal (5, 6).
28. Referential Opacity/Obliquity
"Ross believes that Nadia's dog is lost",
"Ross believes that Nadia's spaniel is lost".
(2) cannot be inferred from (1) because the first sentence does not explicitly say that Ross knows the breed of Nadia's dog. An individual's personal knowledge is necessarily circumscribed and it cannot be assumed that objective facts are subjectively known by everyone. Reference to subjective knowledge occurs in words like `believe', `say', `think' that report indirect discourse and mental activity.
29. Conventional Ambiguity
"The chickens are ready to eat",
"the Boston train is on track nine",
`human offspring', `human race', `human dog', 'human consumption', `human experimentation';
`dog meat'.
Both of the first two examples contain ambiguous elements. `Ready to eat' can be interpreted as an idiom meaning -fully-prepared- or as a construction meaning -hungry-. In the second example, modifying `train' with `Boston' leaves it unclear whether the train is going to or has come from that city. These ambiguities arise from conventions which we routinely assume from the context when we read either statement. However the first example should not really be ambiguous at all. Its -fully-prepared- sense can only be understood by misreading it; to make that statement in correct grammar would be "The chickens are ready to be eaten". Advertising practice has made `ready to eat' mean ready-to-be-eaten.
The -to- or -from- confusion in the second example is based on a similar assumption, but one that has not been formally taught through the media. `Boston' in this case could also mean -made-in-Boston-, -named-after-Boston- or even -owned-by-the-Boston-subway-corporation-. The directional senses are selected as the ones to be confused about because movement and travel is the first thing most people associate with trains. Disambiguation comes (if it does) through knowledge of the timetable.
The five `human' examples all use the adjective to communicate different relationships between its object and human beings: offspring-of-humans, race-composed-of-humans, consumption-by-humans, experimentation-on-humans, dog- with-human-traits,. Similarly, although both words in the last example are individually unambiguous, when collocated it is unclear whether they mean meat-to-feed-to-the-dog or flesh-of-the-dog.
30. Complementary or Copulative Verbs
"John is quiet".
The twenty copula are a special case of relationship. They are used in a statement to allow an entity to be qualified in potentially elaborate manner and not to express a formal relationship. Notwithstanding this, these verbs do qualify their complement's .existence.
A reasonably exhaustive list of copulative verbs is: subjective complements: `be', `become', `grow', `get', `feel', `seem', `appear', `taste', `sound', `smell',`look', `remain', `act', `go', `turn', `make'; objective complements: `make', `prove', `imagine', `consider'.
31. Uninstantiated Reference
"They're always watching",
"It rained yesterday".
The referents for `they' and `it' are supplied by convention and not context; none will be found in the narrative.
32. Meaning Shift through Extension
"The oscilloscope Tom used to fix",
"The oscilloscope Tom used to fix the radio"
This example has elements of garden pathing and idiom, but the construction it is based on may be unique. In the first sentence `oscilloscope' is the direct object; in the second, it is an instrument and `radio' is the direct object. Winograd (1983) believes different senses of `used to' are employed. In the first sentence the phrase, which he represents as `useta', expresses past tense idiomatically; in the second, instrumentality.
33. Reference through Synonym
"My sister's kids are at the store again. Those three boys sure like candy",
"The man left the house. The sun was shining. It was warm and windless. A bird sang. He felt good".
We understand that `those three boys' refers to `kids' because the two noun phrases are highly synonymous (though not exactly so; little girls can be kids too). Less synonymous pairs and reference through negated antonyms are harder to understand.
Synonyms and long range together are a recipe for expensive processing in cases of reference. If `the man' or `Mr. Smith' is substituted for `he' in the last sentence of the second example, the previous four sentences or 15 words would need to be tested to catch the intended reference.
Discourse Semantic Cases
34. Diexis in Dialogue
"`This is the man. He put it there'",
"John pointed to his damaged car and said, `look at that! Do you think I can see through that? It'll run, but that tire'll be torn to shreds in a mile if I do.'".
The first reported statement cannot be understood without context. In the second we infer that the car's windshield has been made opaque by cracks and stars, and that although the tires are not flat, part of the fender or frame is pressing against one of them.
Spoken words can identify things in the scene which are not mentioned in the utterance. The same effect can be accomplished in a written narrative only if the identified item is explicitly or implicitly introduced in the vicinity of the dialogue.
35. Dialogue Attribution
"I did it." "No, you didn't." "Yes, he did!"
The content of these three sentences imply three speakers rather than two individuals taking turns which would otherwise have been assumed.
Written English does not identify speakers of directly-quoted dialogue once they have been introduced. Quotation marks are used to delimit utterances and speakers are assumed to take turns. This convention becomes problematic when a conversation involves more than two people; we then must rely on our models of the participants' interests, motivations and perspectives to determine attributions. There is a distinct risk of misinterpretation in a dialogue among three or more people.
36. Speech Acts
"Could you pass the salt shaker?",
"She turned to her two suitors who stood there gift in hand and said, `I'll take the flowers'."
Speech acts can be divided into those in which the locutionary sense is incongruous with the context and those in which it is congruous. The first case, illustrated by (1) covers what is commonly known by Austin's (1962) term. The locutor's interest in the auditor's physical capacity is irrelevant to his current activity of eating.
The second case is more contextual. In (2) all parties know that the woman is expressing a preference for more than a gift. Although the second utterance is superficially similar to a metonymy (in that the speech act consists of the flowers being understood to stand for the man who is offering them), it differs in that its sense is meant literally; no doubt the woman really expects to and does receive the flowers. Her utterance is therefore not solely figurative.
37. de re vs de dicto Scope
"He confidently walked down the garden path",
"He apparently walked down the garden path."
'Confidently' applies to the act of walking within the statement and elaborates its sense; `apparently' applies to the entire statement and qualifies the proposition it communicates. The former is a de re usage and the latter de dicto. A frame to distinguish between these two cases translates candidate words to their cognate noun form: "It is apparent that ...". Any statement that can be sensibly paraphrased in this way is de dicto.
37. Modal and Hypothetical Existence
"It might snow",
"he ought to come",
"Were he here, he would disagree'.
Modal verbs are a closed set which qualify the possibility, capability and necessity of a specific act or class of acts occurring (1). In addition, modals qualify the volition, intention, prediction, permission and obligation of an animate agent to perform an act (2).
The subjunctive mood is used to express statements which have an imaginary or hypothetical existence (3).
38. Necessary vs Accidental Consequence
"Tom entered the room. The apple fell off the table",
"Tom slammed his fist on the table. The apple fell off of it",
"Tom looked at the river. The apple fell off the table",
"Tom is at work. The apple fell off the table".
In the absence of explicit connectors like `because', `however' and `since' (colloquial) we must employ commonsense knowledge to infer that causal consequence accompanies temporal sequence. The first example is open to either a necessary or an accidental interpretation. In my view it leans slightly towards causality because it goes to the trouble of mentioning the events. The second example is clearly to be understood causally. The third and fourth examples involves events, and a state and an event respectively. Readers are not likely to think that the apple's fall in either case proceeded from Tom's action or state.
39. Indeterminate Set Membership
"The family went to a play. It was so poorly acted that some of the audience booed. Others left and some people began to talk among themselves".
After reading the above sentences, it is impossible to know which of the family members booed, left, began to talk among themselves or continued to watch the play. Language utterances routinely define groups and perform operations on them: 1) conjunction (`the gang and I'), 2) disjunction ("It's them or us"), 3) member and 4) subset identification and selection ("the whole family was among the audience ...", `one of the staff'. These opeations result in the specification of groups or individuatls whose identties are unknown. Language users trust that they will be told the composition of a group when this is important.

english Berkesan..

banyak Yang g confidence kalo harus  ngomong bahasa inggris.... takut salah pronounciaton lah, grammarlah.... de el el..


english is not our mother tongue... we are nonative... so nyantai aja lagi.... kalo tokh salah'' namanya juga belajar..right...


hmmm ne several tips how to get impression for english learning :





1. Never Study A Single English Word
That’s right, do not memorize words. Native speakers do not learn English by remembering single words. Native speakers learn phrases.
2. Never Study Grammar
Right now. Stop. Put away your grammar books and textbooks. Grammar rules teach you to think about English, you want to speak automatically– without thinking!
3. Learn With Your Ears, Not Your Eyes
Spend most of your study time listening- that is the key to great speaking.
4. Slow, Deep Learning Is Best
Its not enough to know a definition. Its not enough to remember for a test. You must put the word deep into your brain. To speak English easily, you must repeat each lesson many times.
5. Use Point Of View Mini-Stories
You must learn grammar by listening to real English. The best way is to listen to the same story… told in different times (points of view): Past, Perfect, Present, Future. How do you do this? Easy! Find a story or article in the present tense. Then ask your native speaker tutor to write it again in the Past, with Perfect tenses, and in the Future. Finally, ask him to read and record these stories for you.
6. Only Use Real English Lessons & Materials
How do you learn Real English? It’s easy. Stop using textbooks. Instead, listen only to real English movies, TV shows, audio books, audio articles, stories, and talk radio shows. Use real English materials.
7. Listen and Answer Mini-Stories (not Listen and Repeat)
Because the teacher constantly and quickly asks easy questions, you don’t have time to think about grammar. You just immediately shout a couple of words– which teaches you to respond faster, and faster, and faster.






Hmmmm enjoy aja Man...!!!








Friday, July 23, 2010

The Phrase in English Form and Function in the English Phrase Words are the constituent elements of the next rank, phrases. At the phrase rank,

The Phrase in English
Form and Function in the English Phrase


Words are the constituent elements of the next rank, phrases. At the phrase rank, we discover that it is possible to analyze each structure in more than one way. To study this phenomenon more closely, we will look at phrase structure in English. English is a language with five classes of phrases, noun phrases, verb phrases, adjective phrases, adverb phrases, and prepositional phrases.
The Noun Phrase
Like all phrases, the constituents of the English noun phrase can be analyzed into both functional constituents and formal constituents. From a functional point of view, the noun phrase has four major components, occurring in a fixed order:
• the determinative, that constituent which determines the reference of the noun phrase in its linguistic or situational context;
• premodification, which comprises all the modifying or describing constituents before the head, other than the determiners;
• the head, around which the other constituents cluster; and
• postmodification, those which comprise all the modifying constituents placed after the head.
In the diagram below, notice that each functional component of a noun phrase (NP) can be further subclassified as we trace the diagram from left to right until we find that we have form classes (of the kind we discussed above) filling each constituent category.

Depending on the context of situation, we choose determiners and modifiers according to our needs in identifying and specifying the referent of the NP. Sometimes we need several determiners and modifiers to clarify the referent (all my books in that box); sometimes we need none at all (Liz).
That diagram is one way to represent the dual nature of a phrase. Each phrase, remember, is a merger of both form and function, and, as complex as it looks, the diagram illustrates only some of the complexities of the noun phrase in English. (For a more thorough treatment, see Halliday 1994 and Quirk et al. 1985.) Another way to illustrate some of the possible arrangements of form and function in the noun phrase is presented in the table below.
Some Examples of the Noun Phrase in English

FUNCTION Determiner Premodifier Head Postmodifier

(a) lions
E (b) the young
X (c) the information age
A (d) each of the children
M (e) some badly needed time with the family
P (f) this conclusion to the story
L (g) all my children
E (h) several new mystery books which we recently enjoyed
S (i) such a marvelous data bank filled with information
(j) a better person than I

FORMS Pronoun Participle Noun Prepositional Phrase
Article Noun Adjective Relative Clause
Quantifier Adjective Phrase Pronoun Nonfinite Clause
Complementation
Notice that several forms classes can be "reused." For example, in the noun phrase it is possible to use quantifiers to function as pre-determiners or as post-determiners. This kind of "recycling" is known as recursion. Notice also that phrases and even whole clauses can be "recycled" into the noun phrase. This process of placing a phrase of clause within another phrase of clause is called embedding. It is through the processes of recursion and embedding that we are able to take a finite number of forms (words and phrases) and construct an infinite number of expressions. Furthermore, embedding also allows us to construct an infinitely long structure, in theory anyway.
For example, the nursery rhyme "The House That Jack Built" plays on the process of embedding in English noun phrases. The nursery rhyme is one sentence that continuously grows by embedding more and more relative clauses as postmodifiers in the noun phrase that ends the sentence:
This is the house that Jack built.
This is the malt that lay in the house that Jack built.
This is the mouse that ate the malt that lay in the house that Jack built.
This is the cat that scared the mouse that ate the malt hat lay in the house that Jack built.
This is the dog that chased the cat that scared the mouse that ate the malt that lay in the house that Jack built.
This is the boy who loves the dog that chased the cat that scared the mouse that ate the malt that lay in the house that Jack built.
And so on. In theory, we could go on forever because language relies so heavily on embedding.
The Verb Phrase
The verb phrase (VP) in English has a noticeably different structure, since the information it carries about mood, tense, modality, aspect, and voice is quite different from the information carried by a noun phrase. The verb phrase has two functional parts,
• the auxiliary, a grammatical morpheme carrying information about mood, tense, modality, and voice; and
• the main verb, a lexical morpheme carrying its lexical information and, usually, an inflection.

The mood system in English is divided into four subcategories.

The indicative mood 'indicates;' that is, it conveys to the listener/reader that the speaker/writer is making a statement, referring to the real world in an honest, direct, relevant way. The majority of our expressions are indicative in mood. Speakers signal the indicative mood by using word order: when the auxiliaries take their "usual" position following the 'subject,' we interpret the clause as being in the indicative mood.
Philosophers of language, like H. P. Grice, have done some of the most interesting linguistics of recent years, explicating the meanings of the indicative mood in English by examining how people use language in conversation. After studying a series of conversations in different contexts, he developed the following generalizations or "rules of conversation" that help explain much about how we interpret our language in the indicative mood. Grice (1975) pointed out the participants in a conversation expect each other to be cooperative, to say something true and to the point, and not to be withholding any relevant information.
Specifically, Grice's maxims, or "rules," are the following:
MAXIM OF QUANTITY
a. Make your contribution as informative as is required.
b. Do not make your contribution more informative than is required.
MAXIM OF QUALITY
c. Do not say what you believe to be false.
d. Do not say that for which you lack adequate evidence.
MAXIM OF RELATION
e. Be relevant.
MAXIM OF MANNER
f. Avoid obscurity of expression.
g. Avoid ambiguity.
h. Be brief.
i. Be orderly.
Look at the conversation between A and B below. The maxims of quantity and relation are at work in B's response, like principles guiding our indicative interpretation of the fragment.
A: When will you stop by?
B: Sometime after dinner.
What maxims are at work in the following conversations between C and D?
C: I'm hungry.
D: I've got five dollars.
The reply D makes is only sensible if we assume that D is following the maxim of relation (that D is being relevant to C's statement of hunger) and the maxim of manner (that D being brief).
The interrogative mood signals the speakers' desire for information, that they are asking a question, that they are 'interrogating' the listeners. The interrogative is marked by starting a clause with an auxiliary verb or an interrogative pronoun.
(1) Can Liz do that?
(2) What will Liz do?
The imperative mood express the speakers' sense of a command, request, or exhortation - an imperative. Speakers signal the imperative mood by using a base form of the verb in clause-initial position.
(3) Do that!
(4) Be here by 8:00 pm.
The subjunctive mood express the speakers' sense of the unlikely, a wish, a prayer, a hope. The subjunctive describes the state of affairs as speakers wish or hope them to be. It describes hypothetical situations, "some other world," the irreal. Speakers signal the subjunctive by beginning subordinate clauses with an auxiliary or by using subordinators that overtly mark hypothetical conditions.
(5) Had I known you were coming, I'd have baked a cake.
(6) If I were a millionaire, I'd endow an award in your honor.
The subjunctive is also marked in the verb phrase by the use of subject-verb concord, as in (6), where the singular subject I is matched with the plural verb were. Base forms of verbs can also signal the subjunctive.
(7) I suggest that Ms. Jones reconsider her decision.
(8) The administration insists that no one be exempted from the placement exams.
Finally, the base form is also used in several older, formulaic subjunctive expressions that have survived in the modern language.
(9) God save the King.
(10) Heaven forbid it should snow again.
Tense systems mark time. Tense is an inflection on the verb that indicates the time reference of the expression. In English, tense is marked on the first verb of the verb phrase. All verbs marked for tense are called 'finite' verbs, while verb forms that do not carry a tense inflection (such as participles) are called 'nonfinite' verbs.
English has two tenses, really. On most English verbs, the -s inflection marks the present tense, and the -ed inflection marks the past tense. Verbs using both the -s and -ed forms are known as 'regular' verbs in that those verbs employ the most common, most 'productive' inflection to mark time, as in helps/helped, hopes/hoped, loves/loved. 'Irregular verbs,' on the other hand, fall into seven subcategories, employing a number of inflections (such as -en for the participle inflection as in written, taken, stolen) or in some classes employing no participle inflections at all (such as put or cut as in Liz cuts the cake/Liz cut the cake/Liz has cut the cake).
It is really better to refer to the present tense in English as the 'nonpast,' since English uses the present tense to refer to many different time frames. Consider the sentences below, for example, where all the verbs are marked for the present tense, but the time reference varies considerably from example to example:
(11) Emily is tired today.
(12) Emily is leaving soon.
(13) Emily is clever.
Sentence (11) indeed does refer to the present time frame, but sentence (12) seems to refer to the near future, although it is marked for "present" tense. Sentence (13) is altogether different: its time frame is expansive, referring to the past, present, and future simultaneously.
What people commonly call the 'future' in English is really part of the modality system of the language. English speakers use modal auxiliary verbs (like will) or phrasal verbs (like is going to - often phonologically reduced to gonna) to refer to the future.
The modality system expresses the speakers' sense of obligation, volition, probability, permission, and ability. Modality is signaled by the use of a modal auxiliary verb and the use of a base verb, modal + base.
(14) Liz {must/should} go. ("obligation")
(15) Liz will stop that immediately! ("volition")
(16) Liz {may/might} go. ("probability")
(17) Liz {can/may} go. ("permission")
(18) Liz {can/could} do it ("ability")
The modal will also expresses a 'future' sense.
(19) Liz will do it tomorrow. ("future")
The modality system also includes a class of auxiliary verbs called semi-auxiliaries (or semi-modals or quasi-modals). These idiomatic verbs and phrasal verbs express modal (and sometimes aspectual) meaning. They often (but not always) take the form of BE+PARTICIPLE+to. These semi-auxiliaries resemble auxiliaries in that they have some equivalence of meaning. Syntactically, the semi-auxiliaries are a mixed group. The semi-auxiliaries starting with be do not need do-support in negation or question formation, but most of the others do employ do-support for those syntactic processes. (See the discussion of do-support below.)

Aspect signals either the completion or the continuation of the process indicated by the verb in English. The perfect aspect expresses the speakers' sense of completion, the speakers' sense that the process expressed by the verb has been "perfected," to use the older sense of the word. The perfect aspect is signaled by the use of a form of the auxiliary have and the -ed participle, have + V-ed. (Remember that some verbs are irregular, with irregular participle forms.)
(20) Liz has gone already.
The progressive aspect expresses the speakers' sense that the process expressed by the verb continues, covers a period of time, and is in some way relevant to the present moment. The progressive aspect is signaled by the use of a form of the auxiliary be and the -ing participle, be + V-ing.
(21) Liz is doing the best work ever.
Voice systems allows speakers to view the action of the sentence in different ways without changing the facts involved. English has two voices, active and passive. In the examples below, it is possible to see the event from the perspective of the 'agent' (the conscious "doer" of the action - that is active voice), as in (22), or from the perspective of the 'goal' (the "receiver" of the action - that is passive voice), as in (23).
(22) Liz encourages Emily. (active)
(23) Emily is encouraged by Liz. (passive)
The passive voice is signaled by the use of a form of be and the -ed participle, be + V-ed.
Lastly, English also employs the verb do to function as a supporting auxiliary in verb phrases that require an auxiliary for certain grammatical processes but which lack some other auxiliary already discussed. Consider (24) for example where it would be impossible to signal the interrogative mood without the support of the do auxiliary, as in (25).
(24) Emily sleeps well at night.
(25) Does Emily sleep well at night?
To signal the interrogative mood, remember, the auxiliary verb occurs in clause-initial position. However, if the clause has not auxiliary verb, as (24) does not, then do-support provides the necessary auxiliary, as in (25). Notice too that do has all the hallmarks of an auxiliary: not only does it occur in clause-initial position as other auxiliaries do in the interrogative mood, but it also is marked for tense like all first verbs in the finite English verb phrase.
Another way to illustrate some of the relationships between form and function in the verb phrase is presented in the table below.
Some Examples of the Verb Phrase in English

FUNCTION Auxiliaries Main Verb

(a) do believe
E (b) can go
X (c) may have gone
A (d) is going
M (e) has been waiting
P (f) might have been waiting
L (g) were hired
E (h) are being hired
S (i) should be trying
(j) might have been being interviewed

FORM Modal Perfect Progressive Passive Auxiliary Support Main Verb
The Adjective Phrase
The adjective phrase in English has four functional constituents,
• premodification, those modifying, describing, or qualifying constituents which precede the head;
• the head, which is an adjective or participle serving as the focus of the phrase;
• postmodification, that modifying constituent which follows the head; and
• complementation, (the major subcategory of postmodification here) that constituent which follows any postmodification and completes the specification of a meaning implied by the head.

To see some examples of adjective phrases, examine the table below.
Some Examples of the Adjective Phrase in English

FUNCTION Premodifier Head Postmodifier

(a) happy
E (b) excited indeed
X (c) partly cloudy
A (d) young in spirit
M (e) very energetic for his age
P (f) so extremely sweet
L (g) too good to be true
E (h) hot enough for me
S (i) quite worried about the results of the test
(j) unusually sunny for this time of year

FORM Adverb
Adverb Adjective Prepositional Phrase
Adverb Phrase Infinitive Clause
Notice that the order of constituents in the adjective phrase, like all other phrase structures in English, is relatively fixed, helping us determine the constituent elements.
The Adverb Phrase
The adverb phrase in English is nearly identical to the adjective phrase, with only the expected changes in form. In the adverb phrase, an adverb functions as head.

To see some examples of adverb phrases, examine the table.
Some Examples of the Adverb Phrase in English

FUNCTION Premodifier Head Postmodifier

(a) quietly
E (b) quite honestly
X (c) very hard indeed
A (d) however
M (e) really early
P (f) so very well indeed
L (g) too quickly to see well
E (h) likely enough for us
S (i) formerly of Cincinnati
(j) more easily than ever

FORM Adverb
Adverb Adverb Prepositional Phrase
Adverb Phrase Infinitive Clause
The Prepositional Phrase
The last structure for us to study at the phrase rank is the prepositional phrase. This phrase is a 'nonheaded' construction in English since no one constituent functions as the center of the phrase, the center on which the other elements depend. Instead, the structure is divided into two functional components - the preposition followed by its complement. In general, a prepositional phrase expresses a relationship between the complement of the preposition and some other constituent of the sentence. Diagrammatically, the structure of the prepositional phrase looks like this:

The table below illustrates some of the possible structures found in the English prepositional phrase.
Some Examples of the Prepositional Phrase in English

FUNCTION Preposition Complement

(a) for now
E (b) with her
X (c) in time
A (d) next to the table
M (e) into the thick of things
P (f) by the time that you read this
L (g) before slipping off to sleep
E (h) after running more than 500 miles in one week
S (i) to whomever it may concern
(j) from what I can see

FORM Preposition Adverb
Pronoun
Noun
Noun Phrase
-ing Clause
Relative Clause
________________________________________
REFERENCES
Halliday, Michael A. K.
1994 Introduction to Functional Grammar 2nd edition, London: Edward Arnold.
Quirk, R., S. Greenbaum, G. Leech, and J. Svartvik.
1985 A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language, London: Longman.

© 1995, 2010 Daniel Kies. All rights reserved.
Document URL: http://papyr.com/hypertextbooks/grammar/phrase.htm
Last revision: 05/21/2010 18:40:53

study about syntax

Structural Analysis of English Syntax
 Definitions
 Inflectional Paradigms
 Derivational Paradigms
 Intonation Patterns
 Position or Word Order
 Form Class Words
Nouns –Class I, Pronouns, Verbs –Class II, Adjectives –Class III, Adverbs –Class IV
 Function Words (determiners, conjunctions, others)
 Syntactic Combinations, Phrase Analysis, Immediate Constituent Analysis, and Sentence Formulas

What is syntax?
 The forms that English words may be given and the sequences in which they are arranged with other words to express larger, more complex meanings make up the syntactic pattern or system of English.
 The syntax of English –the third level of grammar and the third level of analysis –deals with the more complex combinations of linguistic forms. Identification of a word class is not, then, a matter of “What do these words mean?” but “How do they fit into a pattern?” “What forms will they take?” and “How do they behave in combination with other forms?” (1999, Herndon)
 The way in which words are put together to form constructions (American Heritage Dictionary)

Paradigms
The devices used by structuralists for establishing word classes in English include consideration of ways in which certain types of words can be grouped into sets, called paradigms, on the basis of the inflectional and derivational affixes that they will take.
1. Inflectional paradigms
They are sets of forms. Each set is made up of a base form (singular), plus whatever morphemic changes –either the addition of suffixes or sound changes or both –may be used to adapt the base form to certain functions without changing the lexical meaning.
(1999, Herndon)
For example, the inflectional paradigm for the class form (NOUNS) is made up as follows.
Base
(singular) Base Form +
plural Base Form +
possessive Base Form +
Possessive plural
teacher teachers teacher’s
teacher’s desk teachers’
teachers’ rights
student students student’s students’
Nouns – inflectional paradigms
2. Derivational paradigms
Derivational paradigms are made up of sets of endings that may be attached to bases that may shift their lexical meaning or part of speech or both. Some examples of noun-marking derivational suffixes are –hood, -ship, -ness, and –ment. Words having these endings are recognized, even in isolation, as nouns. (1999, Herndon)














Other aspects of syntax
 Intonation Patterns –contrasts made by the differences of stress, pitch, and juncture often identify a form as belonging to one word class or another. For example the difference between the noun contract and the verb contract, is determined by differences in intonation pattern.
(1999, Herndon)
 Position or Word Order – word classes are usually identifiable on the basis of where they appear in a given sentence. Many words are not recognizable as a single part of speech when they are met in isolation. According to Herndon, we do not need the structuralists to prove this, but rather to rely on Webster’s Collegiate Dictionary, which lists the word round as adjective, noun, transitive verb, preposition, and adverb. In order to isolate the definition that you seek, you must have the word in a context.

Function Words
 Some words in English may not make use of the structural paradigms. They have no inflectional or derivational endings.
 They perform a function in the system –outside of the grammatical relationships they signify, they have little or no meaning.
 The categories of function words are often called closed classes because new forms are rarely, if ever, added to them.
 Function words represent only a few hundred of the more than half a million words in English. (1999, Herndon)












Form Class Words
1. Nouns –Class I Words



1. Inflectional paradigm –generally speaking, nouns are forms that will accept inflections. (slide # 5)
2. Derivational paradigms –many forms may be recognized as nouns on the basis of various noun-marking derivational suffixes added either to bound bases or to other words –often words belonging to other classes. There are literally dozens of these endings. For example, -er, -or, and –ment adapt verbs to use as nouns;
Examples: verbs + derivational suffix = noun
work + er = worker
play + er = player
stimulate + or = stimulator
govern + ment = government
3. Intonation Pattern – differences of stress may distinguish nouns from verbs (slide # 7). Heavier stress on the first syllable almost always signals a noun; heavier stress on the second signals a verb. noun -súspect / verb -suspéct
4. Position or Word Order
Nouns fill certain characteristic positions in relation to other parts of speech. The most obvious is that just before the verb.
Examples: The _____ is here. These _____ are beautiful!
5. Function Words –In English, noun determiners immediately precede nouns or precede them with certain words in between. Some noun determiners never appear except when followed by a noun and invariably signal its coming. These are the articles the, a, and an and the possessive pronouns my, your, our, and their. Other pronouns are quite frequently used as determiners, but have other functions as well. These are the demonstratives this, that, these, and those and the other possessive pronouns, his, her, and its.


Pronouns
 When considered a separate class, pronouns are Class II words, but most school texts consider them a subcategory of nouns.
 In contrast to nouns, pronouns constitute a closed class –no new pronouns have been added to English for hundreds of years. If anything, the class has become smaller instead, as few speakers now make use of the forms thee, thou, thy, and thine.
 Personal pronouns fall into an inflectional paradigm that is similar to, but not exactly like, that for nouns. Forms show both number and the possessive case, but they also show gender and the nominative and objective cases.
Example: he / his / him (see enclosures)
 Pronouns, in most cases are identifiable by the ability of each to substitute for a type of noun or noun phrase.
(1999, Herndon)

3. Verbs Class II Words
a. Inflectional Paradigm –English verbs commonly have five forms , the base form and four inflected forms. These inflections are the present 3rd person singular, the past, the present participle, and the past participle form. (see example below)
• The present 3rd singular is similar in many ways to the noun inflections
• The past tense, or preterit, is commonly formed with the –ed ending, but there are several irregular allomorphs.
• The present participle is formed by an –ing suffix.
• The past participle makes use of –ed and –en endings or internal vowel changes. In a class by itself in many ways is the verb be, which has eight inflected forms (be, am, is, are, was, were, being, been)
base 3rd sing. past present part. past part.
eat eats ate eating eaten

3. Verbs –cont.
b. Derivational Paradigm
Some verbs are marked by suffixes such as the –ate ending added to bound bases and nouns, the –ize added to bound bases, nouns, and adjectives, and the –fy added to bound bases, nouns, and adjectives, and the prefix –en added to nouns and some other verbs.
Examples: summarize, beautify, locate, etc.
c. Intonation Pattern See contrasts with nouns marked by intonation
d. Position or Word Order
Some positions mark verbs. Verbs commonly occupy the first position in requests, a position between two nouns or pronouns, or between noun and adjective or adverb. Here’s a simple set of test frames for verbs.
1. The child may___ something.
2. The children ___ friendly.
3. ___ you ____ me that?
e. Function Words – function words that work with verbs are the various forms of have and be and the modals can, may, should, will, and others. (1999, Herndon)


4. Adjectives – Class III Words

a. Inflectional Paradigms
True adjectives commonly show comparative and superlative degrees by adding –er and –est inflections. Derived adjectives make use of the function words more and most for this purpose.
b. Derivational Paradigms
True adjectives fit into derivational patterns with nouns formed by adding the suffix –ness to true adjectives and adverbs formed by adding the suffix –ly to the same adjectives. (1999, Herndon)
happy-happiness-happily
b. (Cont.) Adjectives are derived from other words by adding such endings as –y, ic, and –ous to nouns and bound bases; -ful and –less to nouns; -able,-ent, and –ive to verbs and bound bases.
1. greed greedy
2. class classic
3. danger dangerous
4. need needful
5. home homeless
6. manage manageable
7. differ different
8. persuade persuasive

4. Adverbs – Class IV Words
Many adverbs share several structural distinctions with adjectives

a. Inflectional Paradigm
In a few cases adverbs admit the comparative and superlative degree endings (er, est), usually they use more and most. Some adverbs have a base form that also serves as an adjective (fast, hard). In this case the class will depend upon other structural devices. (1999, Herndon)
b. Derivational Paradigm –the most common adverb-marking suffix is the –ly added to adjectives (common + ly), (soft+ ly), (bare + ly). .
There are other combinations.

c. Intonation Patterns
The intonation patterns of larger structures often show adverbs patterning closely with verbs, in contrast to adjectives which usually pattern with nouns.
d. Word Order
Most adverbs in English are extremely mobile. Various types may fill any of several positions or positional combinations, but almost all can fill the position following a noun-verb-complement sequence like the following.
The boy ate his cookies _____.
(1999, Herndon)

Function Words

 Determiners – The workings of the determiner class of function words is described in some detail under the form class with which they appear, the nouns or class I words. The most commonly used members are the, a, an, and some.
 Auxiliary Verbs – Forms of the auxiliaries have and be work with various inflected forms of verbs. Modals are usually considered a subcategory because their operation is somewhat different from that of have and be. Other auxiliaries are forms of get and do.
 Qualifiers – They work with both adjectives and adverbs. Some of the most frequently used are more, most, very, quite, rather, and somewhat.
 Prepositions – They introduce modifying or qualifying phrases set apart by intonation pattern and the presence of the preposition form. They indicate the relation of words with other words. (eg. location, direction, time, etc.)
 Conjunctions – They always work as coordinators of linguistics forms or syntactic units having equal value. The two most frequently used are and and but.
 Subordinators – they connect dependent clauses and include words such as because, after, although, unless, and so on, as well as the relative pronouns who, whose, which, and that.
 Interrogatives – they operate in the formation of questions and include words such as when, where, why, how and so on, as well as the interrogative pronouns who, which, and what

Syntactic Combinations

 When smaller structures enter into combinations, some consideration must be given to the relationship holding between them within the combination. For example, “Birds fly” consists of structures commonly called noun and verb.
 The combination is a larger structure called a sentence. Within the sentence both words have a structure and a function.
 Analysis of any larger structure involves sorting its parts into types of smaller structures and identifying the functions performed in the combination.
 Structural grammarians vary somewhat in the methods used to analyze complex grammatical structures in English. (1999, Herndon)
 This chapter discusses three of the principal methodologies used by grammarians. (1999, Herndon)
a. phrase analysis b. immediate constituent analysis c. sentence formulas

Phrase Analysis

 One of these methods begins with consideration of word “clusters that are set apart on the basis of the intonation pattern that they show. A group of words appearing between well-defined junctures is described as a phrase or cluster.
 The principal word in each phrase is called the head word.
 In general, phrases function as units in larger structures, and they fall into groups based on the type of function the unit performs.
 Noun phrases, verb phrases, and various types of modifying or qualifying phrases –adjectival, adverbial, prepositional, and so on –may be defined.
 Analysis may then be made of relationships holding between the various types when they appear in various combinations. Finally, clause and sentence types may be defined. (1999, Herndon)

Immediate Constituent Analysis (IC)

 The second method and perhaps the most widely used means of dealing with English syntax is the IC.
 Sentences are divided into their principal parts or immediate constituents.
 Each of these is then divided and subdivided until the ultimate constituents of the sentence are reached.
1. The boys / shyly touched the puppy.
shyly touched / the puppy.
The / boys / shyly / touched / the / puppy.
Small puppies / are fat and frisky.
are / fat and frisky.
Small / puppies / are/ fat / and/ frisky.
 Further cuts might even divide the plural morphemes from boy and puppy, the inflectional –ed from touch and the ly from shy.

ICs –structures and functions

 In ICs the relationship is analyzed and identified after each cut is made. The first cut yields structures that function as subject and predicate.
The boys shyly touched the puppy.
Structures: (NP) noun phrase (VP) verb phrase
Functions: Subject Predicate

 The second cut yields structures that function as verbal element and complement (or object) within the predicate
The boys shyly touched the puppy.
Structures: VP NP
Functions: Verbal Element Complement (Object)

ICs – final cut

The boys shyly touched the puppy.
S. Det. Noun Adverb Verb Det. Noun
F. Mod. Head Modifier Head Mod. Head
 Among other things, this type of analysis gives rise to the practice of referring to noun-headed and verb-headed structures when speaking of phrases. (1999, Herndon)


Sentence Formulas

 The third method is one that begins with a consideration of basic sentence patterns and proceeds to analyze the relationships between the different parts of the patterns
 Each of the parts of a very simple sentence can be expanded in various ways so that more complex sentence patterns –and more complex layers of relationships –are produced
 Sentence patterns of the simplest noun-verb-noun, noun-verb-adjective types are considered first.
 The sentence parts are designated by the numbers and letters assigned to their form class or function word groups. (1999, Herndon)

 A short sample list might include these groups:

Form Class Words
1. Noun or pronoun
2. Verb
3. Adjective
4. Adverb
Function Words
D. Determiner
A. Auxiliary
Q. Qualifier
P. Preposition

SUMMARY

 Structural analysis of English syntax divides the parts of speech into form class words and function words. Categories of form class words are identified on the basis of the following criteria:
1. inflectional paradigm
2. derivational paradigm
3. intonation pattern
4. word order
5. function words that work with them
 Several methods of phrase and sentence analysis have been used by structuralists. The most influential one is called immediate constituent (IC) analysis.

reffences

(1999, Herndon)